In October 1960, a 26-year-old Jane Goodall was walking through a rocky outcrop in what is now Tanzania’s Gombe National Park when she saw movement in the long grasses ahead. Peering through her binoculars, Goodall watched as an older male chimpanzee with distinctive silver facial hair sat on an earthen mound. Again and again, the animal pushed a stem of grass into a hole in the mound, withdrew the stem and picked something off it with his mouth. When he left, Goodall tried mimicking the same motion and saw termites clinging to the stem.
Goodall later observed a chimpanzee picking a small leafy twig and removing its leaves before using it to fish for termites. In that moment, she knew she had seen not only evidence of chimp tool use but also the beginnings of tool making—two behaviors previously thought to be exclusively human.
“My observations at Gombe challenged human uniqueness,” Goodall later wrote in her memoir Reason For Hope. “And whenever that happens there is always a violent scientific and theological uproar.”
Goodall Relied on Keen, Open-Minded Observation
Goodall had no college degree or scientific training when she traveled from her native England to Africa several years earlier. Instead, she brought a lifelong love of animals and the desire to observe lions, elephants, giraffes, monkeys and other creatures in their native habitat.
In Nairobi, Kenya, she met celebrated paleoanthropologist Dr. Louis Leakey, who hired her as his personal secretary at the local natural history museum.
Known for his pioneering research into human evolution at Olduvai Gorge in Tanzania, Leakey spoke with Goodall about his interest in chimpanzees, gorillas and orangutans, the closest living relatives of humans. Studies of ape behavior had begun early in the 20th century, but until then research had mainly been done on chimpanzees in captivity.
“In those days, work in the natural habitat was frowned upon as unscientific: Only laboratory approaches provided the controls required for conclusive science,” the Dutch-American primatologist Frans de Waal wrote in Nature in 2011 of the early years of chimpanzee research.
Leakey wanted to send someone to study chimpanzees in the wild to better understand how humans’ Stone Age ancestors may have behaved. He saw Goodall’s lack of formal training as an advantage, as she would presumably be more open-minded and less bound to traditional scientific wisdom about primate behavior.